留言板

尊敬的读者、作者、审稿人, 关于本刊的投稿、审稿、编辑和出版的任何问题, 您可以本页添加留言。我们将尽快给您答复。谢谢您的支持!

姓名
邮箱
手机号码
标题
留言内容
验证码

中等强度体操运动对5~6岁幼儿执行功能的影响

张建华 卢嘉程 刘敏 闫小凡

张建华, 卢嘉程, 刘敏, 闫小凡. 中等强度体操运动对5~6岁幼儿执行功能的影响[J]. 中国学校卫生, 2024, 45(3): 326-329. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2024082
引用本文: 张建华, 卢嘉程, 刘敏, 闫小凡. 中等强度体操运动对5~6岁幼儿执行功能的影响[J]. 中国学校卫生, 2024, 45(3): 326-329. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2024082
ZHANG Jianhua, LU Jiacheng, LIU Min, YAN Xiaofan. Effects of moderate-intensity gymnastics on executive function in children aged 5 to 6 years[J]. CHINESE JOURNAL OF SCHOOL HEALTH, 2024, 45(3): 326-329. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2024082
Citation: ZHANG Jianhua, LU Jiacheng, LIU Min, YAN Xiaofan. Effects of moderate-intensity gymnastics on executive function in children aged 5 to 6 years[J]. CHINESE JOURNAL OF SCHOOL HEALTH, 2024, 45(3): 326-329. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2024082

中等强度体操运动对5~6岁幼儿执行功能的影响

doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2024082
基金项目: 

国家社会科学基金项目 19BTY115

详细信息
    作者简介:

    张建华(1989-), 男, 河南商丘人, 硕士, 讲师, 主要研究方向为运动与脑科学

  • 利益冲突声明  所有作者声明无利益冲突。
  • 中图分类号: R179  G83  Q427

Effects of moderate-intensity gymnastics on executive function in children aged 5 to 6 years

  • 摘要:   目的  探讨中等强度体操运动对5~6岁幼儿执行功能发展的影响, 为幼儿执行功能的运动干预提供参考依据。  方法  从商丘某私立幼儿园整群随机抽取3个大班63名5~6岁儿童为试验对象, 根据性别分层, 采用随机数字表法将幼儿分为干预组(31人)和对照组(32人)。干预组进行每周3次, 每次60 min, 共12周的体操运动, 并使用Polar H10心率带全程监控运动强度; 对照组儿童在相同时间内在教室观看教育卡通片。试验前后对幼儿的抑制控制、工作记忆和认知灵活性进行评估。  结果  干预前两组幼儿在抑制控制、工作记忆和认知灵活性任务中的表现差异均无统计学意义(P值均>0.05)。协方差分析结果显示, 干预组与对照组在抑制控制、工作记忆和认知灵活性中的反应时和准确性差异均有统计学意义(F值分别为6.84, 4.50, 4.87, 6.11, 3.74, 5.06, P值均 < 0.05)。干预效果呈现小到中等效应量(d=0.17~0.74)。  结论  中等强度体操运动对幼儿执行功能产生了小到中等效应的提升作用。未来研究设计可引入脑成像技术, 以深入探究体操运动对幼儿大脑结构和执行功能的影响机制。
    1)  利益冲突声明  所有作者声明无利益冲突。
  • 表  1  体操动作内容及训练安排

    Table  1.   Contents of gymnastic movements and training arrangements

    课部分 项目 动作名称及量化要求 动作要求 时间/min
    热身部分 通过小组游戏方式进行,如模仿小动物走、跑、跳等 10
    体操训练 自由操 (1)猩猩爬—8 m—一组 既快速,又协调 45
    (2)兔子跳—5个—一组 有腾空,有远度
    (3)前滚翻—3个—一组 团身紧,动作圆
    (4)侧滚翻—5 m—一组 身体直,动作顺
    低平衡木 (1)直立提踵行走—起点到终点—一组 重心稳,身体直
    (2)两腿依次前踢—起点到终点—一组 腿过膝,脚绷直
    (3)依次转身行进—起点到终点—一组 重心稳,身体直
    (4)双人对立行进—起点到终点—一组 重心稳,不掉木
    跳箱 (1)5 m助跑上板,落在10 cm海绵垫上—一组 四肢协调,踏板有力
    (2)5 m助跑上板,落在30 cm海绵垫上—一组 四肢协调,踏板有力
    (3)5 m助跑上板,在50 cm海绵箱上成蹲撑接挺身下—一组 四肢协调,踏板有力
    (4)5 m助跑上板,在50 cm海绵垫上成半蹲接挺身下—一组 四肢协调,踏板有力
    小蹦床 (1)上下垂直跳—6个—一组 四肢协调,节奏清晰
    (2)左右开合跳—6个—一组 四肢协调,节奏清晰
    (3)前后分腿跳—6个—一组 四肢协调,节奏清晰
    (4)团身吸腿跳—3个—一组 四肢协调,节奏清晰
    放松部分 以上下肢拉伸为主 5
    下载: 导出CSV

    表  2  干预前后干预组和对照组执行功能任务表现比较(x±s)

    Table  2.   Comparison of executive functional task performance between the intervention group and control group pre- and post-intervention(x±s)

    干预前后 组别 人数 统计值 抑制控制反应时/ms 抑制控制准确性/% 工作记忆反应时/ms 工作记忆准确性/% 认知灵活性反应时/ms 认知灵活性准确性/%
    干预前 干预组 31 863.17±13.57 52.55±13.79 945.62±237.84 69.78±10.54 1 163.21±50.85 66.73±7.23
    对照组 32 860.19±22.53 50.66±8.20 912.97±225.25 68.23±6.01 1 153.11±31.51 65.51±7.51
    t 0.64 0.66 0.56 0.71 0.94 0.66
    P 0.53 0.51 0.58 0.48 0.35 0.52
    干预后 干预组 31 782.88±24.29 73.86±7.26 719.90±119.99 86.36±7.63 1 042.39±72.75 76.45±9.48
    对照组 32 805.13±23.74 67.02±8.22 833.55±177.87 80.50±9.39 1 091.29±49.42 69.59±7.66
    F 6.84 6.11 4.50 3.74 4.87 5.06
    P <0.01 <0.01 0.02 0.03 0.01 0.01
    下载: 导出CSV
  • [1] FUNAHASHI S. Neuronal mechanisms of executive control by the prefrontal cortex[J]. Neurosci Res, 2001, 39(2): 147-165. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(00)00224-8
    [2] JUSIENÊ R, RAKICKIENÊ L, BREIDOKIENÊ R, et al. Executive function and screen-based media use in preschool children[J]. Infant Child Dev, 2020, 29(1): e2173. doi: 10.1002/icd.2173
    [3] ZELAZO P D, ANDERSON J E, RICHLER J, et al. Ⅱ. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring executive function and attention[J]. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev, 2013, 78(4): 16-33. doi: 10.1111/mono.12032
    [4] RIGGS N R, SPRUIJT-METZ D, SAKUMA K L, et al. Executive cognitive function and food intake in children[J]. J Nutr Educ Behav, 2010, 42(6): 398-403. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2009.11.003
    [5] ESPY K A, MCDIARMID M M, CWIK M F, et al. The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children[J]. Dev Neuropsychol, 2010, 26(1): 465-486.
    [6] MORIGUCHI Y. The early development of executive function and its relation to social interaction: a brief review[J]. Front Psychol, 2014(1): 1-6.
    [7] JACKA F N, PASCO J A, WILLIAMS L J, et al. Lower levels of physical activity in childhood associated with adult depression[J]. J Sci Med Sport, 2011, 14(3): 222-226. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2010.10.458
    [8] DIAMOND A. Activities and programs that improve children's executive functions[J]. Curr Dir Psychol Sci, 2012, 21(5): 335-341. doi: 10.1177/0963721412453722
    [9] LENROOT R K, SCHMITT J E, ORDAZ S J, et al. Differences in genetic and environmental influences on the human cerebral cortex associated with development during childhood and adolescence[J]. Hum Brain Mapp, 2009, 30(1): 163-174. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20494
    [10] ALESI M, GIORDANO G, GIACCONE M, et al. Effects of the enriched sports activities-program on executive functions in Italian children[J]. J Funct Morphol Kinesiol, 2020, 5(2): 26-38. doi: 10.3390/jfmk5020026
    [11] PANHELOVA M. Complex development of cognitive processes and physical abilities of children of preschool age in the process of motor activity[J]. J Int Soc Sports Nutr, 2020, 9(1): 74-84.
    [12] 王异芳, 苏彦捷. 心理理论的执行功能假说[J]. 中国临床康复, 2004(3): 524-526. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-XDKF200403074.htm

    WANG Y F, SU Y J. The hypothesis of executive function in psychological theory[J]. Chin J Clin Rehabil, 2004(3): 524-526. (in Chinese) https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-XDKF200403074.htm
    [13] 蔡春先, 张运亮, 朱艳彤, 等. 间歇运动干预对儿童执行功能的影响及其延迟效益研究[J]. 首都体育学院学报, 2021, 33(5): 541-548. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-BTSF202105010.htm

    CAI C X, ZHANG Y L, ZHU Y T, et al. Investigation into the impact of intermittent exercise intervention on children's executive function and its enduring benefits[J]. J Cap Univ Phys Educ Sprots, 2021, 33(5): 541-548. (in Chinese) https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-BTSF202105010.htm
    [14] MCMORRIS T, HALE B J. Differential effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy of cognition: a Meta-analytical investigation[J]. Brain Cogn, 2012, 80(3): 338-351. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.09.001
    [15] LEE J S, BOAFO A, GREENHAM S, et al. The effect of high-intensity interval training on inhibitory control in adolescents hospitalized for a mental illness[J]. Ment Health Phys Act, 2019, 17: 1-9.
    [16] LIVESEY D, KEEN J, ROUSE J, et al. The relationship between measures of executive function, motor performance and externalising behaviour in 5-and 6-year-old children[J]. Hum Mov Sci, 2006, 25(1): 50-64. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2005.10.008
    [17] WILLOUGHBY M, BLAIR C. Test-retest reliability of a new executive function battery for use in early childhood[J]. Child Neuropsychol, 2011, 17(6): 564-579. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2011.554390
    [18] DAVIDSON M C, AMSO D, ANDERSON, et al. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching[J]. Neuropsychologia, 2006, 44(11): 2037-2078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006
    [19] TYLER P D, DU H, FENG M, et al. Assessment of intensive care unit laboratory values that differ from reference ranges and association with patient mortality and length of stay[J]. JAMA Netw Open, 2018, 1(7): e184521-e184521. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.4521
    [20] THORELL L B, LINDQVIST S, BERGMAN N S, et al. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children[J]. Dev Sci, 2009, 12(1): 106-113. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00745.x
    [21] RÖTHLISBERGER M, NEUENSCHWANDER R, CIMELI P, et al. Improving executive functions in 5-and 6-year-olds: evaluation of a small group intervention in prekindergarten and kindergarten children[J]. Infant Child Dev, 2012, 21(4): 411-429. doi: 10.1002/icd.752
    [22] TRAVERSO L, VITERBORI P, USAI M C. Improving executive function in childhood: evaluation of a training intervention for 5-year-old children[J]. Front Psychol, 2015, 6(5): 525-538.
    [23] BAI J, HUANG H, OUYANG H. Effects of group-play moderate to vigorous intensity physical activity intervention on executive function and motor skills in 4-to 5-year-old preschoolers: a pilot cluster randomized controlled trial[J]. Front Psychol, 2022, 13: 847785. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.847785
    [24] EGGER F, BENZING V, CONZELMANN A, et al. Boost your brain, while having a break! The effects of long-term cognitively engaging physical activity breaks on children's executive functions and academic achievement[J]. PLoS One, 2019, 14(3): e0212482. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212482
    [25] KOORTS H, TIMPERIO A, ARUNDELL L, et al. Is sport enough? Contribution of sport to overall moderate-to vigorous-intensity physical activity among adolescents[J]. J Sci Med Sport, 2019, 22(10): 1119-1124. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2019.06.009
    [26] O'BRIEN M W, KIMMERLY D S, MEKARI S. Greater habitual moderate-to-vigorous physical activity is associated with better executive function and higher prefrontal oxygenation in older adults[J]. Geroscience, 2021, 43(6): 2707-2718. doi: 10.1007/s11357-021-00391-5
    [27] MCNEILL J, HOWARD S J, VELLA S A, et al. Physical activity and modified organized sport among preschool children: associations with cognitive and psychosocial health[J]. Ment Health Phys Act, 2018, 15: 45-52. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.07.001
    [28] COOK C J, HOWARD S J, SCERIF G, et al. Associations of physical activity and gross motor skills with executive function in preschool children from low-income South African settings[J]. Dev Sci, 2019, 22(5): e12820. doi: 10.1111/desc.12820
    [29] MEKARI S, EARLE M, MARTINS R, et al. Effect of high intensity interval training compared to continuous training on cognitive performance in young healthy adults: a pilot study[J]. Brain Sci, 2020, 10(2): 81-93. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10020081
    [30] VOSS M W, CARR L J, CLARK R, et al. Revenge of the "sit" Ⅱ: does lifestyle impact neuronal and cognitive health through distinct mechanisms associated with sedentary behavior and physical activity?[J]. Ment Health Phys Act, 2014, 7(1): 9-24. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.01.001
    [31] BEST J R, MILLER P H. A developmental perspective on executive function[J]. Child Dev, 2010, 81(6): 1641-1660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x
    [32] BENZING V, CHANG Y K, SCHMIDT M. Acute physical activity enhances executive functions in children with ADHD[J]. Sci Rep, 2018, 8(1): 12382. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-30067-8
    [33] BEST J R. Exergaming immediately enhances children's executive function[J]. Dev Psychol, 2012, 48(5): 1501-1510. doi: 10.1037/a0026648
  • 加载中
表(2)
计量
  • 文章访问数:  84
  • HTML全文浏览量:  32
  • PDF下载量:  27
  • 被引次数: 0
出版历程
  • 收稿日期:  2023-10-10
  • 修回日期:  2024-01-12
  • 网络出版日期:  2024-03-29
  • 刊出日期:  2024-03-25

目录

    /

    返回文章
    返回